There is a growing recognition of how animals respond to the affective states of other animals, including the show of empathy, a state once thought to be unique to primates...A key question concerns the nature of the rats' motivations—the affective and cognitive underpinnings of their “empathy.” ...Future research needs to untangle whether empathic responses in mammals arise more from higher cognitive or lower affective brain functions, or some combination of these (see the figure). Human brain imaging studies of empathy suggest both are involved, especially in coping with the distress of others. But solid neurobiologically based evolutionary evidence, both bottom-up and top-down, is so far lacking.
Figure legend - One concept of how mammalian brains generate empathic responses at different levels is shown. Primary emotional processes, where sources of empathy may arise (i.e., feeling what other organisms are feeling), coordinate with secondary-process learning and memory mechanisms (i.e., knowing what others are feeling). Both of these then interact with higher mental processes, which can exert a variety of top-down influences on the regulation of empathic tendencies (i.e., desires to respond compassionately to others' distress).
The layering of evolutionary progressions is evident in the human brain. The deepest midbrain and hypothalamic regions mediate primary-process, instinctual affects. More recently evolved subcortical regions, among them basal ganglia, amygdala, and nucleus accumbens, help promote higher cognitive activities through learning and memory. Although we currently look to mirror-neuron zones of the neocortex for evidence of the highest mind functions such as compassion, empathic tendencies are surely also promoted by the more ancient primary-process emotional networks that are essential foundations for mental life. For example, a primal form of “empathy” is mothers' exquisite sensitivity to crying babies. Might crying access those systems in mothers' brains that are known to mediate separation anxiety in young animals? Perhaps affective urges for maternal caregiving are triggered as mothers' brains experience psychological pain engendered by their infants' cries. It may be that empathic coordination of social motivations is mediated by emotional resonances among nearby animals, allowing receivers to experience the emotions of transmitters. At such deep affective levels, emotional states may reverberate among animals, with no need for learned rerepresentations arising from mirror neurons. Mammals may have intrinsic abilities to resonate with the pains and joys of nearby others through primal emotional contagion.
Much deep-brain research remains to be done to understand the degree to which mammalian empathy is achieved more through higher social-cognitive processes or primal affective processes in the brain. Simplified models of empathy, as in mice and rats, offer new inroads for understanding our own social-emotional nature and nurture. Such knowledge may eventually help us promote nurturant behaviors in humans.
This blog reports new ideas and work on mind, brain, behavior, psychology, and politics - as well as random curious stuff. (Try the Dynamic Views at top of right column.)
Wednesday, December 14, 2011
Heirarchies of empathy in the brain
Following the previous post I thought it would be useful to pass on more of Panskepp's review, which provides a more general description:
Blog Categories:
animal behavior,
mirror neurons
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
Thanks, a very helpful article. I am autistic and have serious problems with sensory overload. Like other autistic people I have problems with my lack of empathy. For example I never had much of a romantic life in large part due to my complete inability to read signals from women and perceive that they were interested in me. But seven years ago I did begin having rare flashes of empathy so I suspect that my brain has partially figured out how to complete the neural circuitry to support empathy. But the idea of a hierarchy of empathy does seem very likely to me. At the moment I am trapped in my apartment due to an extreme case of sensory overload. Oddly enough I am also experiencing a serious problem with emotional overload. I am unable to view examples of powerful emotions with out needing to immediately disconnect. Neither can I read something if it contains emotional content. Yet some part of my brain is obviously involved in empathy or else I wouldn't even be aware that I was dealing with an expression of strong emotion. So I must have empathy operating within the more primitive part of the brain. My problems with processing powerful negative emotions, while not being able to perceive more nuanced and subtle emotions is deeply consistent with this idea. And nothing gives me greater problems than to hear a child cry, I am absolutely unable to tolerate it. I imagine in most humans the interlinking from the mammalian brain that provide feedback to the primitive brain allow most people to quickly become alert when a child cries and then to moderate the experience to a manageable level. Thanks.
ReplyDeleteFascinating self observations on your part. Thanks for putting them down.
ReplyDeleteTough reading but very enlightening. I wonder if many of these go awry since we built a society where people can manage without the help of others. There seems to be a lot of energy on the loose, being shifted from personal to global causes. Or to animals, for example.
ReplyDelete@Daniel Geisler I recognise a process within me after menopause and seven children that I go on sensory and emotional overload very quickly, it's like the empathy circuitry has drained dry. At times it wants to jump into action and then it fails, as I don't have the reserves to support such moves any more. I think the worst thing is the feeling of being a quirk, when everyone harps on findings that people are happiest with friends and family, socializing and empathizing. Arrrggghh.