Two long-standing observations about human cognitive behaviour provide Michael Shermer with the fundamentals of his account of how people form beliefs. One is the brain's readiness to perceive patterns even in random phenomena. The other is its readiness to nominate agency — intentional action — as the cause of natural events.
Both explain belief-formation in general, not just religious or supernaturalistic belief. Shermer, however, has a particular interest in the latter, and much of his absorbing and comprehensive book addresses the widespread human inclination to believe in gods, ghosts, aliens, conspiracies and the importance of coincidences.
The important point, Shermer says, is that we form our beliefs first and then look for evidence in support of them afterwards. He gives the names 'patternicity' and 'agenticity' to the brain's pattern-seeking and agency-attributing propensities, respectively. These underlie the diverse reasons why we form particular beliefs from subjective, personal and emotional promptings, in social and historical environments that influence their content.
As a 'belief engine', the brain is always seeking to find meaning in the information that pours into it. Once it has constructed a belief, it rationalizes it with explanations, almost always after the event. The brain thus becomes invested in the beliefs, and reinforces them by looking for supporting evidence while blinding itself to anything contrary. Shermer describes this process as “belief-dependent realism” — what we believe determines our reality, not the other way around.
He offers an evolution-based analysis of why people are prone to forming supernatural beliefs based on patternicity and agenticity. Our ancestors did well to wonder whether rustling in the grass indicated a predator, even if it was just the breeze. Spotting a significant pattern in the data may have meant an intentional agent was about to pounce.
Problems arise when thinking like this is unconstrained, he says. Passionate investment in beliefs can lead to intolerance and conflict, as history tragically attests. Shermer gives chilling examples of how dangerous belief can be when it is maintained against all evidence; this is especially true in pseudo-science, exemplified by the death of a ten-year-old girl who suffocated during the cruel 'attachment therapy' once briefly popular in the United States in the late 1990s.
Shermer's account implies that we are far from being rational and deliberative thinkers, as the Enlightenment painted us. Patternicity leads us to see significance in mere 'noise' as well as in meaningful data; agenticity makes us ascribe purpose to the source of those meanings. How did we ever arrive at more objective and organized knowledge of the world? How do we tell the difference between noise and data?
His answer is science. “Despite the subjectivity of our psychologies, relatively objective knowledge is available,” Shermer writes. This is right, although common sense and experience surely did much to make our ancestors conform to the objective facts long before experimental science came into being; they would not have survived otherwise.
Powerful support for Shermer's analysis emerges from accounts he gives of highly respected scientists who hold religious beliefs, such as US geneticist Francis Collins. Although religious scientists are few, they are an interesting phenomenon, exhibiting the impermeability of the internal barrier that allows simultaneous commitments to science and faith. This remark will be regarded as outrageous by believing scientists, who think that they are as rational in their temples as in their laboratories, but scarcely any of them would accept the challenge to mount a controlled experiment to test the major claims of their faith, such as asking the deity to regrow a severed limb for an accident victim.
Shermer deals with the idea that theistic belief is an evolved, hard-wired phenomenon, an idea that is fashionable at present. The existence of atheists is partial evidence against it. More so is that the god-believing religions are very young in historical terms; they seem to have developed after and perhaps because of agriculture and associated settled urban life, and are therefore less than 10,000 years old.
The animism that preceded these religions, and which survives today in some traditional societies such as those of New Guinea and the Kalahari Desert, is fully explained by Shermer's agenticity concept. It is not religion but proto-science — an attempt to explain natural phenomena by analogy with the one causative power our ancestors knew well: their own agency. Instead of developing into science, this doubtless degenerated into superstition in the hands of emerging priestly castes or for other reasons, but it does not suggest a 'god gene' of the kind supposed for history's young religions with their monarchical deities.
This stimulating book summarizes what is likely to prove the right view of how our brains secrete religious and superstitious belief. Knowledge is power: the corrective of the scientific method, one hopes, can rescue us from ourselves in this respect.
This blog reports new ideas and work on mind, brain, behavior, psychology, and politics - as well as random curious stuff. (Try the Dynamic Views at top of right column.)
Friday, July 08, 2011
How we form beliefs
A.C. Grayling offers a review of Michael Shermer's latest book "The Believing Brain: From Ghosts and Gods to Politics and Conspiracies — How We Construct Beliefs and Reinforce Them as Truths," which looks like a fascinating read. Shermer is a psychology professor, the founder of Skeptic magazine and resident sceptical columnist for Scientific American (I've done MindBlog posts on several of these columns). Grayling does such a concise job of summing up Shermer's main points that I pass on chunks of the review here (Sigh...like many of you, I suspect, I read many more reviews of books than actual books.)
One reason many of us, like you, read many more reviews of books than actual books may be that authors seem to feel compelled to write 250-400 pages on a subject that could be adequately elucidated in 25-50 pages. But then who would pay $35 for a 25 page book (even though it might give you all the information you need, in a lot less reading time)? Perhaps Internet publishing could solve that problem.
ReplyDelete'This stimulating book summarizes what is likely to prove the right view of how our brains secrete religious and superstitious belief.'
ReplyDeleteIs this Grayling looking for evidence to confirm a belief he has already formed?
Just asking.
Looks like a fascinating read indeed. Nice excerpt.
ReplyDeleteThis all sounds great, except it leaves out the most important component — dopamine. We form beliefs and turn them into “truths” based on a desire to score dopamine. Shermer wrote his book to score dopamine. Grayling reviewed it to score dopamine. You summarized Grayling’s review to score dopamine. And I’m taking the time to comment to score dopamine. And if anyone enjoys or gets upset by my comment it will be because of dopamine.
ReplyDeleteSorry, but I should have included this link in the previous post. It’s from Dr. Robert Sapolskyk and it’s brilliant. (I’m not affiliated with Sapolskyk).
ReplyDeletehttp://www.dailymotion.com/video/xh6ceu_dopamine-jackpot-sapolsky-on-the-science-of-pleasure_news
Thanks for you comments, I'm going to point to them in a quickie post.
ReplyDeleteI postulate that science(the methodology you speak of) is just another religion, the "rules" of which are just another construct of human thinking. Every marketer worth their salt is aware of the "hypnotic" effect of numbers, especially if introduced as statistics.
ReplyDeleteThere is no need to go back 10,000 years. All religions extant today have roots in the historic era. There is really no evidence that any of the polytheistic religions that preceded them were much older than the Chacholithic ca. 4,000 years BCE. We really have no knowledge of religious practice before then that is any more valid than what we infer about animist hunter-gatherer religious practice.
ReplyDeleteExcellent review. Michael Shermer is a brilliant author. I've read some of his books and highly recommend them. And no his points can't be summed up in 25 pages for it's his examples that make his fascinating points hit home. Warning though: his books are meant for the open-minded for it will definitely make you challenge your beliefs. It did mine.
ReplyDeleteIt's interesting how when people begin relationships and they see value in them. They find reasons to support that relationship. When the relationship however is falling apart or they aren't getting the same type of value from that relationship. They rationalize reasons that this relationship is bad. It's like when someone is dating someone and talks about how nice their partner works then when they break up says that their partner was mean. We constantly rationale are beliefs so we have confidence in our decisions.
ReplyDeleteIt's interesting how when people begin relationships and they see value in them. They find reasons to support that relationship. When the relationship however is falling apart or they aren't getting the same type of value from that relationship. They rationalize reasons that this relationship is bad. It's like when someone is dating someone and talks about how nice their partner works then when they break up says that their partner was mean. We constantly rationale are beliefs so we have confidence in our decisions.
ReplyDelete